Goto

Collaborating Authors

 Country


The Kernel Trick for Distances

Neural Information Processing Systems

This is done by identifying a class of kernels which can be represented as norm-based distances in Hilbert spaces. It turns out that common kernel algorithms, such as SVMs and kernel PCA, are actually really distance based algorithms and can be run with that class of kernels, too. As well as providing a useful new insight into how these algorithms work, the present work can form the basis for conceiving new algorithms. 1 Introduction One of the crucial ingredients of SVMs is the so-called kernel trick for the computation of dot products in high-dimensional feature spaces using simple functions defined on pairs of input patterns. This trick allows the formulation of nonlinear variants of any algorithm that can be cast in terms of dot products, SVMs being but the most prominent example [13, 8]. Although the mathematical result underlying the kernel trick is almost a century old [6], it was only much later [1, 3,13] that it was made fruitful for the machine learning community. Kernel methods have since led to interesting generalizations of learning algorithms and to successful real-world applications. The present paper attempts to extend the utility of the kernel trick by looking at the problem of which kernels can be used to compute distances in feature spaces. Again, the underlying mathematical results, mainly due to Schoenberg, have been known for a while [7]; some of them have already attracted interest in the kernel methods community in various contexts [11, 5, 15].


Vicinal Risk Minimization

Neural Information Processing Systems

The Vicinal Risk Minimization principle establishes a bridge between generative models and methods derived from the Structural Risk Minimization Principlesuch as Support Vector Machines or Statistical Regularization. Weexplain how VRM provides a framework which integrates a number of existing algorithms, such as Parzen windows, Support Vector Machines, Ridge Regression, Constrained Logistic Classifiers and Tangent-Prop. We then show how the approach implies new algorithms forsolving problems usually associated with generative models. New algorithms are described for dealing with pattern recognition problems with very different pattern distributions and dealing with unlabeled data. Preliminary empirical results are presented.


On Reversing Jensen's Inequality

Neural Information Processing Systems

Jensen's inequality is a powerful mathematical tool and one of the workhorses in statistical learning. Its applications therein include the EM algorithm, Bayesian estimation and Bayesian inference. Jensen computes simplelower bounds on otherwise intractable quantities such as products of sums and latent log-likelihoods. This simplification then permits operationslike integration and maximization. Quite often (i.e. in discriminative learning) upper bounds are needed as well. We derive and prove an efficient analytic inequality that provides such variational upper bounds. This inequality holds for latent variable mixtures of exponential family distributions and thus spans a wide range of contemporary statistical models.We also discuss applications of the upper bounds including maximum conditional likelihood, large margin discriminative models and conditional Bayesian inference. Convergence, efficiency and prediction results are shown.


Competition and Arbors in Ocular Dominance

Neural Information Processing Systems

Hebbian and competitive Hebbian algorithms are almost ubiquitous in modeling pattern formation in cortical development. We analyse in theoretical detaila particular model (adapted from Piepenbrock & Obermayer, 1999) for the development of Id stripe-like patterns, which places competitive and interactive cortical influences, and free and restricted initial arborisationonto a common footing. 1 Introduction Cats, many species of monkeys, and humans exibit ocular dominance stripes, which are alternating areas of primary visual cortex devoted to input from (the thalamic relay associated with)just one or the other eye (see Erwin et aI, 1995; Miller, 1996; Swindale, 1996 for reviews of theory and data). These well-known fingerprint patterns have been a seductive targetfor models of cortical pattern formation because of the mix of competition and cooperation they suggest. A wealth of synaptic adaptation algorithms has been suggested to account for them (and also the concomitant refinement of the topography of the map between the eyes and the cortex), many of which are based on forms of Hebbian learning. Critical issues for the models are the degree of correlation between inputs from the eyes, the nature of the initial arborisation of the axonal inputs, the degree and form of cortical competition, and the nature of synaptic saturation (preventing weights from changing sign or getting too large) and normalisation (allowing cortical and/or thalamic cells to support only a certain total synaptic weight).


Kernel-Based Reinforcement Learning in Average-Cost Problems: An Application to Optimal Portfolio Choice

Neural Information Processing Systems

Many approaches to reinforcement learning combine neural networks orother parametric function approximators with a form of temporal-difference learning to estimate the value function of a Markov Decision Process. A significant disadvantage of those procedures isthat the resulting learning algorithms are frequently unstable. In this work, we present a new, kernel-based approach to reinforcement learning which overcomes this difficulty and provably converges to a unique solution. By contrast to existing algorithms, our method can also be shown to be consistent in the sense that its costs converge to the optimal costs asymptotically. Our focus is on learning in an average-cost framework and on a practical application tothe optimal portfolio choice problem. 1 Introduction Temporal-difference (TD) learning has been applied successfully to many real-world applications that can be formulated as discrete state Markov Decision Processes (MDPs) with unknown transition probabilities.



Active Inference in Concept Learning

Neural Information Processing Systems

People are active experimenters, not just passive observers, constantly seeking new information relevant to their goals. A reasonable approach to active information gathering is to ask questions and conduct experiments that maximize the expected information gain, given current beliefs (Fedorov 1972, MacKay 1992, Oaksford & Chater 1994). In this paper we present results on an exploratory experiment designed to study people's active information gathering behavior on a concept learning task (Tenenbaum 2000). The results of the experiment are analyzed in terms of the expected information gain of the questions asked by subjects. In scientific inquiry and in everyday life, people seek out information relevant to perceptual and cognitive tasks.


Spike-Timing-Dependent Learning for Oscillatory Networks

Neural Information Processing Systems

The model structure is an abstrac- tion of the hippocampus or the olfactory cortex. We propose a simple generalized Hebbian rule, using temporal-activity-dependent LTP and LTD, to encode both magnitudes and phases of oscillatory patterns into the synapses in the network. After learning, the model responds resonantly to inputs which have been learned (or, for networks which operate essentially linearly, to linear combinations of learned inputs), but negligibly to other input patterns. Encoding both amplitude and phase enhances computational capacity, for which the price is having to learn both the excitatory-to-excitatory and the excitatory-to-inhibitory connections. Our model puts contraints on the form of the learning kernal A(r) that should be experimenally observed, e.g., for small oscillation frequencies, it requires that the overall LTP dominates the overall LTD, but this requirement should be modified if the stored oscillations are of high frequencies.


Beyond Maximum Likelihood and Density Estimation: A Sample-Based Criterion for Unsupervised Learning of Complex Models

Neural Information Processing Systems

Two well known classes of unsupervised procedures that can be cast in this manner are generative and recoding models. In a generative unsupervised framework, the environment generates training exampleswhich we will refer to as observations-by sampling from one distribution; the other distribution is embodied in the model. Examples of generative frameworks are mixtures of Gaussians (MoG) [2], factor analysis [4], and Boltzmann machines [8]. In the recoding unsupervised framework, the model transforms points from an obser- vation space to an output space, and the output distribution is compared either to a reference distribution or to a distribution derived from the output distribution.


Structure Learning in Human Causal Induction

Neural Information Processing Systems

We use graphical models to explore the question of how people learn simple causalrelationships from data. The two leading psychological theories canboth be seen as estimating the parameters of a fixed graph. We argue that a complete account of causal induction should also consider how people learn the underlying causal graph structure, and we propose to model this inductive process as a Bayesian inference. Our argument is supported through the discussion of three data sets. 1 Introduction Causality plays a central role in human mental life. Our behavior depends upon our understanding ofthe causal structure of our environment, and we are remarkably good at inferring causation from mere observation. Constructing formal models of causal induction is currently a major focus of attention in computer science [7], psychology [3,6], and philosophy [5].This paper attempts to connect these literatures, by framing the debate between two major psychological theories in the computational language of graphical models. We show that existing theories equate human causal induction with maximum likelihood parameter estimationon a fixed graphical structure, and we argue that to fully account for human behavioral data, we must also postulate that people make Bayesian inferences about the underlying causal graph structure itself.