Goto

Collaborating Authors

 Edelman, Shimon


A Productive, Systematic Framework for the Representation of Visual Structure

Neural Information Processing Systems

For example, priming in a subliminal perception task was found to be confined to a quadrant of the visual field [16]. The notion that the representation of an object may be tied to a particular location in the visual field where it is first observed is compatible with the concept of object file, a hypothetical record created by the visual system for every encountered object, which persists as long as the object is observed. Moreover, location (as it figures in the CoF model) should be interpreted relative to the focus of attention, rather than retinotopically [17]. The idea that global relationships (hence, large-scale structure) have precedence over local ones [18], which is central to our approach, has withstood extensive testing in the past two decades. Even with the perceptual salience of the global and local structure equated, subjects are able to process the relations among elements before the elements themselves are identified [19]. More generally, humans are limited in their ability to represent spatial structure, in that the representation of spatial relations requires spatial attention.


A Productive, Systematic Framework for the Representation of Visual Structure

Neural Information Processing Systems

For example, priming in a subliminal perception task was found to be confined to a quadrant of the visual field [16]. The notion that the representation of an object may be tied to a particular location in the visual field where it is first observed is compatible with the concept of object file, a hypothetical record created by the visual system for every encountered object, which persists as long as the object is observed. Moreover, location (as it figures in the CoF model) should be interpreted relative to the focus of attention, rather than retinotopically [17]. The idea that global relationships (hence, large-scale structure) have precedence over local ones [18], which is central to our approach, has withstood extensive testing in the past two decades. Even with the perceptual salience of the global and local structure equated, subjects are able to process the relations among elements before the elements themselves are identified [19]. More generally, humans are limited in their ability to represent spatial structure, in that the representation of spatial relations requires spatial attention.


A Productive, Systematic Framework for the Representation of Visual Structure

Neural Information Processing Systems

For example, priming in a subliminal perception task was found to be confined to a quadrant of the visual field [16]. The notion that the representation of an object may be tied to a particular location in the visual field where it is first observed is compatible with the concept of object file, a hypothetical record created by the visual system for every encountered object, which persists as long as the object is observed. Moreover, location (as it figures in the CoF model) should be interpreted relative to the focus of attention, rather than retinotopically [17]. The idea that global relationships (hence, large-scale structure) have precedence over local ones [18], which is central to our approach, has withstood extensive testing in the past two decades. Even with the perceptual salience of the global and local structure equated, subjects are able to process the relations among elements before the elements themselves are identified [19]. More generally, humans are limited in their ability to represent spatial structure, in that the representation of spatial relations requires spatial attention. For example, visual search is difficult when above below 0. 9


Anatomical origin and computational role of diversity in the response properties of cortical neurons

Neural Information Processing Systems

Our results show that maximal diversity of neuronal response properties is attained when the ratio of dendritic and axonal arbor sizes is equal to 1, a value found in many cortical areas and across species (Lund et al., 1993; Malach, 1994). Maximization of diversity also leads to better performance in systems of receptive fields implementing steerablejshiftable filters, which may be necessary for generating the seemingly continuous range of orientation selectivity found in VI, and in ma.tching spatially distributed signals. This cortical organization principle may, therefore, have the double advantage of accounting for the formation of the cortical columns and the associated patchy projection patterns, and of explaining how systems of receptive fields can support functions such as the generation of precise response tuning from imprecise distributed inputs, and the matching of distributed signals, a problem that arises in visual tasks such as stereopsis, motion processing, and recognition.


Anatomical origin and computational role of diversity in the response properties of cortical neurons

Neural Information Processing Systems

A fundamental feature of cortical architecture is its columnar organization, manifested in the tendency of neurons with similar properties to be organized in columns that run perpendicular to the cortical surface. This organization of the cortex was initially discovered by physiological experiments (Mouncastle, 1957; Hubel and Wiesel, 1962), and subsequently confirmed with the demonstration of histologically defined that axonal projections throughout thecolumns. Tracing experiments have shown tend to be organized in vertically aligned clusters or patches.


3D Object Recognition Using Unsupervised Feature Extraction

Neural Information Processing Systems

Intrator (1990) proposed a feature extraction method that is related to recent statistical theory (Huber, 1985; Friedman, 1987), and is based on a biologically motivated model of neuronal plasticity (Bienenstock et al., 1982). This method has been recently applied to feature extraction in the context of recognizing 3D objects from single 2D views (Intrator and Gold, 1991). Here we describe experiments designed to analyze the nature of the extracted features, and their relevance to the theory and psychophysics of object recognition. 1 Introduction Results of recent computational studies of visual recognition (e.g., Poggio and Edelman, 1990) indicate that the problem of recognition of 3D objects can be effectively reformulated in terms of standard pattern classification theory. According to this approach, an object is represented by a few of its 2D views, encoded as clusters in multidimentional space. Recognition of a novel view is then carried out by interpo-460 3D Object Recognition Using Unsupervised Feature Extraction 461 lating among the stored views in the representation space.


3D Object Recognition Using Unsupervised Feature Extraction

Neural Information Processing Systems

Intrator (1990) proposed a feature extraction method that is related to recent statistical theory (Huber, 1985; Friedman, 1987), and is based on a biologically motivated model of neuronal plasticity (Bienenstock et al., 1982). This method has been recently applied to feature extraction in the context of recognizing 3D objects from single 2D views (Intrator and Gold, 1991). Here we describe experiments designed to analyze the nature of the extracted features, and their relevance to the theory and psychophysics of object recognition. 1 Introduction Results of recent computational studies of visual recognition (e.g., Poggio and Edelman, 1990) indicate that the problem of recognition of 3D objects can be effectively reformulated in terms of standard pattern classification theory. According to this approach, an object is represented by a few of its 2D views, encoded as clusters in multidimentional space. Recognition of a novel view is then carried out by interpo-460 3D Object Recognition Using Unsupervised Feature Extraction 461 lating among the stored views in the representation space.


3D Object Recognition Using Unsupervised Feature Extraction

Neural Information Processing Systems

Gold Center for Neural Science, Brown University Providence, RI 02912, USA Shimon Edelman Dept. of Applied Mathematics and Computer Science, Weizmann Institute of Science, Rehovot 76100, Israel Abstract Intrator (1990) proposed a feature extraction method that is related to recent statistical theory (Huber, 1985; Friedman, 1987), and is based on a biologically motivated model of neuronal plasticity (Bienenstock et al., 1982). This method has been recently applied to feature extraction in the context of recognizing 3D objects from single 2D views (Intrator and Gold, 1991). Here we describe experiments designed to analyze the nature of the extracted features, and their relevance to the theory and psychophysics of object recognition. 1 Introduction Results of recent computational studies of visual recognition (e.g., Poggio and Edelman, 1990)indicate that the problem of recognition of 3D objects can be effectively reformulated in terms of standard pattern classification theory. According to this approach, an object is represented by a few of its 2D views, encoded as clusters in multidimentional space. Recognition of a novel view is then carried out by interpo-460 3D Object Recognition Using Unsupervised Feature Extraction 461 lating among the stored views in the representation space.


A self-organizing multiple-view representation of 3D objects

Neural Information Processing Systems

We demonstrate the ability of a two-layer network of thresholded summation units to support representation of 3D objects in which several distinct 2D views are stored for ea.ch object. Using unsupervised Hebbianrelaxation, the network learned to recognize ten objects from different viewpoints. The training process led to the emergence of compact representations of the specific input views. When tested on novel views of the same objects, the network exhibited asubstantial generalization capability. In simulated psychophysical experiments,the network's behavior was qualitatively similar to that of human subjects.


A self-organizing multiple-view representation of 3D objects

Neural Information Processing Systems

We demonstrate the ability of a two-layer network of thresholded summation units to support representation of 3D objects in which several distinct 2D views are stored for ea.ch object. Using unsupervised Hebbian relaxation, the network learned to recognize ten objects from different viewpoints. The training process led to the emergence of compact representations of the specific input views. When tested on novel views of the same objects, the network exhibited a substantial generalization capability. In simulated psychophysical experiments, the network's behavior was qualitatively similar to that of human subjects.